IB TOK II--Hunt
Objectivity and Subjectivity: the Struggle
One of the first things that struck
me when selecting this particular question was my own knowledge issue regarding
the word “objectively.” As someone who is fond of natural sciences, I have believed
that “objectivity” was synonymous to “accuracy,” for the former can help
increase the effectiveness of an experiment. As psychologist Elaine Vauguan stated:
“the experimental method is one of the most powerful tools for scientists to
identify what are the causes of particular outcomes and effects” (“Psychology:
The Human Experience”). But can an objective experimental method really produce
valid and accurate results in the human sciences?
According to Oxford dictionary, objectivity is defined as “not
being influenced by personal feelings” when considering facts. But by numbing
our emotions, are we not also numbing an entire way of knowing? Following the
premise that humans are emotional by nature, then wouldn’t it be more logical
for the species to be studied subjectively?
It
may seem logical, but in practice subjective research and its qualitative data have
been regarded by some not to be as valid as objective studies and their quantitative
results. This is especially the case when it comes to methods such as
Participant Observation, for “events are interpreted
through the single observer's eyes […] Clearly
one's own views can come in to play” (“Observation: Participant Observation”).
I experienced this conflict with
researcher bias during the “alien” activity, for I had a hard time detaching
from my own perception and schema. Because of this, I constantly found myself
describing scenarios in my journal using particular human jargon, such as “laptop,”
that would have been unknown to a foreigner. Nevertheless, although I was not
successful in repressing my schema while conducting this type of research, I
can’t simply discard the merits of subjective observation, since my obvious
lack of experience with this methodology also affected my ability to take on a
neutral observant role.
For instance, renowned
Anthropologist Wade Davis was very successful in his use of Participant
Observation when studying the people of Chichero
in Peru .
When discussing the traditional run through the mountain region of the area
that he participated in, he concluded: “you go into the mountain as an
individual, but through exhaustion, through sacrifice, you emerge as a
community that has once again reaffirmed its sense of place in the planet” (Davis ). Here, Davis
reveals his perspective on the struggle and fulfillment that takes place during
this traditional event, an insight and understanding which may not have been
obtained had he simply observed the group from a distance.
However,
though these emotions may have been greatly insightful for Davis ’ research, this way of knowing can sometimes
hinder the validity of research, especially when it comes to emotional hijacking.
Take Stanford’s famous Zimbardo prison experiment, for example. As the
psychologist himself stated, he became so caught up in his role as the prison
warden that he was angered when a college asked him about the variables of his
study: “To my surprise, I got really angry at him (…) It wasn’t until much
later that I realized how far into my prison role I was at that point”
(Zimbardo). Thus, this extremely subjective involvement of the researcher with
his own study jeopardized his ability to follow his experimental design.
But
too much objectivity also has its downsides. For instance, when analyzing the
Stanley Milgram experiment objectively, one might agree that his experimental
methodology seems reasonable. But the manner in which the test subjects are
treated is alarming. When noticing that the applied shocks were seemingly unbearable
to the disguised actor, some volunteers asked to stop the experiment. However,
they were forced to continue against their own will (“Obedience to Authority”).
Therefore, the level of objectivity in this case, though not negatively
impacting the results of the study per se, led to an unethical treatment of
test subjects; one that could have perhaps been avoided had the researchers
analyzed the design of study with a more subjective eye.
In
conclusion, returning back to the first part of the question, as it is
evidenced by Participant Observation, we don’t solely study humans objectively.
Nevertheless, the level of objectivity and subjectivity at hand may vary
depending on the subject matter being discussed, as cultural anthropology, for
example, may not require as much experimental research in comparison to areas
in psychology.
But
what about the second part of the question? Should we study humans objectively?
Not necessarily. Taking into consideration the insight that subjective observation
can provide, as seen with Davis ’
work, purely objective research only provides a more quantitative view of human
behavior. Moreover it can also clash with ethical implications. Of course this
doesn’t mean that human studies should be completely subjective, for emotion
and personal schema can also hinder the accuracy of results. Therefore, human study
requires a flexible balance between these two methodologies, for we will never
be able to obtain a holistic understanding of our species if one of these sides
is ignored.
Works
Cited:
Davis, Wade.
"The Worldwide Web of Belief and Ritual." TED:
Ideas worth
Spreading. Web. Feb 2008
"Obedience to
Authority The Experiments by Stanley
Milgram." Stanley
Milgram Obedience
Experiments Authority Study 1974 Psychology. Web. 23 Aug. 2013
"Objectivity." Oxford Dictionary (British
& World English). Web. 23
Aug. 2013.
"Observation: Participant
Observation." UC Davies Psychology. Web.
23 Aug.
2013.
"Psychology -
The Human Experience - Lesson 02 - Research Methods in
Psychology."YouTube. YouTube,
24 Jan. 2013. Web.
Zimbardo, Phil.
"Stanford Prison Experiment." A Simulation Study of the
Psychology
of Imprisonment. N.p.,
n.d. Web. 23 Aug. 2013.
.
Nice work. Good concise elaboration of examples. Impressive control.
ReplyDeleteThis comment has been removed by the author.
ReplyDelete