Currently, the
human sciences aims to imitate the natural sciences in its approach to study –
through an external objective position searching for universal laws independent
of human individuality and subjectivity. But in a field that is founded over
the rickety ground of subjective, emotional and personal factors, which make up
the infamous “human unpredictability” – is this even possible?
Well,
we definitely try to make it so. Harvard’s Project Implicit is an example of a
very objective set of experiments used to explore one of the giants of
subjectivity: group bias. In their different experiments, the programmed software
analyzes time taken to assort pictures of one discrete group to certain words,
the accuracy of the assortment, the answers to a series of multiple-choice
questions – data that essentially turns into numbers in a formula that then
reveals to you that deep inside, you hate overweight people. Objectively, the science
is quite flawless: the independent and dependent variables are defined, the
method is reproducible, and the data robust. But do I really hate fat people? Could
such an objective test reveal something so deeply subjective about myself with
such confidence? The test had me first associate images of overweight
silhouettes to “negative” words, and later asked me to switch from this method
of assortment to one where I had to associate the silhouettes with positive
words. A machine would have little trouble making this transition, but the test
failed to recognize the subjective nature of humans, missing the fact that we
may be influenced by the order in which we are presented information. By overlooking
our subjective factors, such an objective experiment can yield faulty results. Do
not assume that human science chooses to be ignorant of human subjectivity,
however. Economics, like all other human sciences, stare over the chaos of
human unpredictability and subjectivity and must face it. To cope with this
chaos, economics actually translates and generalizes this subjectivity into
cold, hard objective assumptions: “people are rational maximizers,” “people’s
wants are unlimited,” or “people makes choices dependent on future
consequences.” All these assumptions seem safe to make, have actually worked
almost flawlessly in the field, and truthfully, even the natural sciences make
assumptions – the ideal gas law assumes spherical atoms, in mechanics we assume
elastic collisions, in most of our physics problems we ignore the effect of
relativity. But how do we compare the error in objective generalizations made
to cope with subjectivity in the human sciences and the error in
generalizations made in the natural sciences? We saw exactly how drastically the
behavior of each individual in our class differed from one another when we were
asked to abuse the picture of a loved one. Is generalizing such a diverse group
of people the same as generalizing how we consider atoms? Are the consequences
of doing so of the same magnitude in scientific research?
Given
all the implications with an objective approach to understand human nature, we
should ask ourselves, should we look
at human behavior in a purely objective way? The alien anthropologist activity
gave a good insight into this question. By looking into human life, ignoring
any past experience or emotions I have, I reached completely absurd conclusions
about our species however logical they may have been from my objective
observations. Emotion and subjectivity are tools in such cases. By considering
my own experiences, I am able to deduce that humans are not swimming in pools
to undergo osmosis, but for the subjective reason that they enjoy being in the
water. Instead of starting from the ground up when trying to reach conclusions
objectively, we may use what we already know from our own experiences as an
aid. We must be wary, however, as there is a threshold to this idea. Humans try
to relate to others – and other things, even – very often, and although in our
daily lives we can assume someone jumping up and down is happy, an
anthropologist observing a foreign culture must be careful not to. If an
anthropologist allows his personal experience influence his thoughts on why a
culture is the way it is, he is under danger of applying his own culture to
understand another. In other words, the subjective knowledge we carry is not
compatible with all other cultures or individuals. This personal bias, and
subsequently confirmation bias, is only one example of how an emotional
involvement in study may harm scientific results. Through active participation
of an anthropologist in a study for a subjective experience, the researcher is
not only more prone to directly interfere with the study due to subjective reasons,
such as teaching the studied culture to raise a fire above to ground to avoid
burns – consequently altering the development and culture of the society – but
also in the manner that the presence of the researcher may alter the behavior
of the studied.
Weighing
the benefits and hindrances of the purely objective or excessively subjective
study of human behavior, it is clear that a balance must be found between the
two methods. Having organized these thoughts on both, a more appropriate
question to answer next would be “to what extent
should human study be objective or subjective,” as everything must come in good
balance.
This is a great response.
ReplyDeleteThis comment has been removed by the author.
ReplyDelete